0475/2018 - Propósito de vida em adultos e idosos: Revisão integrativa.
Purpose in life in adult and aged people: Integrative review.
Autor:
• Cristina Cristovão Ribeiro - Ribeiro, C.C - <cristinaribeiroft@gmail.com>ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6222-3232
Coautor(es):
• Mônica Sanches Yassuda - Yassuda, M.S - <yassuda@usp.br>ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9182-2450
• Anita Liberalesso Neri - Neri, A.L. - <anitalbn@uol.com>
ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6833-7668
Resumo:
Propósito de vida (PV) é definido como o senso de que a vida tem sentido e intencionalidade, o qual desempenha papel orientador em relação às metas de vida e à tomada de decisões relativas ao uso dos recursos pessoais. Objetivos: reconhecer e analisar dados sobre PV e condições identificadas com bom envelhecimento ou com adaptação positiva no envelhecimento. Método: foi realizada revisão integrativa de publicações de periódicos com avaliação por pares, indexadas nas bases de dados PubMed/Medline, Scopus, Web of Science, PsycInfo e Scielo/BVS/Lilacs, com os termos “purpose in life” and “aging” no título, sem restrição de datas. Resultados: foram selecionados 27 artigos, 22 dos quais conduzidos no âmbito de grandes estudos longitudinais prospectivos, 3 de corte transversal e 2 experimentais, que revelaram associações robustas entre alta pontuação em PV e redução do risco para mortalidade, Doença de Alzheimer, doenças coronarianas e cerebrovasculares, incapacidades e distúrbios de sono; alto PV e desfechos positivos em saúde, cognição, regulação emocional, bem-estar subjetivo e senso de ajustamento; PV como variável moderadora, ou recurso de resiliência, entre riscos e adversidades e boa adaptação. São dados úteis à investigação e à intervenção envolvendo idosos longevos e vulneráveis.Palavras-chave:
metas; satisfação pessoal; longevidade; envelhecimentoAbstract:
Purpose in Life (PiL) is defined as the sense that life has meaning and intentionality, which plays a guiding role in relation to life goals and decision making regarding the use of the personal resources. Objectives: recognize and analyze data on associations between PiL and conditions identified with good aging or with positive adaptation in aging. Method: An integrative review was performed involving articlespeer-reviewed journals, indexed in the PubMed / Medline, Scopus, Web of Science, PsycInfo and Scielo / BVS / Lilacs databases, using the terms \"purpose in life\" and \"aging\" in the title, without restriction of dates. Results: 27 articles were ed, 22 of which were conducted within large prospective longitudinal studies, three cross-sectional studies and two experimental studies, which revealed robust associations between high scores in PiL and reduction of the risk for mortality, Alzheimer\'s disease, coronary and cerebrovascular diseases, disabilities and sleep disorders; high PiL and positive outcomes in health, cognition, emotional regulation, subjective well-being and sense of adjustment; PiL as a moderator variable, or resilience resource, between risks and adversities and good adaptation. These are useful data for research and intervention involving long-lived and vulnerable elderly.Keywords:
goals; personal satisfaction; longevity; agingConteúdo:
Acessar Revista no ScieloOutros idiomas:
Purpose in life in adult and aged people: Integrative review.
Resumo (abstract):
Purpose in Life (PiL) is defined as the sense that life has meaning and intentionality, which plays a guiding role in relation to life goals and decision making regarding the use of the personal resources. Objectives: recognize and analyze data on associations between PiL and conditions identified with good aging or with positive adaptation in aging. Method: An integrative review was performed involving articlespeer-reviewed journals, indexed in the PubMed / Medline, Scopus, Web of Science, PsycInfo and Scielo / BVS / Lilacs databases, using the terms \"purpose in life\" and \"aging\" in the title, without restriction of dates. Results: 27 articles were ed, 22 of which were conducted within large prospective longitudinal studies, three cross-sectional studies and two experimental studies, which revealed robust associations between high scores in PiL and reduction of the risk for mortality, Alzheimer\'s disease, coronary and cerebrovascular diseases, disabilities and sleep disorders; high PiL and positive outcomes in health, cognition, emotional regulation, subjective well-being and sense of adjustment; PiL as a moderator variable, or resilience resource, between risks and adversities and good adaptation. These are useful data for research and intervention involving long-lived and vulnerable elderly.Palavras-chave (keywords):
goals; personal satisfaction; longevity; agingLer versão inglês (english version)
Conteúdo (article):
Purpose in life in adulthood and older adulthood: integrative reviewCristina Cristovão Ribeiro1,2, Mônica Sanches Yassuda1,3, Anita Liberalesso Neri1
1Gerontology, College of Medical Sciences, State University of Campinas, UNICAMP, Campinas, Brazil.
2Physiotherapy, CESUFOZ, Center for Higher Education of Foz do Iguaçu, Foz do Iguaçu, Brazil
3Gerontology, School of Arts, Sciences and Humanities, University of São Paulo, Brazil
Corresponding author: Profa. Dr. Cristina Cristovão Ribeiro
Physical therapist and university professor; PhD in Gerontology from Unicamp; Master in Gerontology from PUC-São Paulo; Specialist in Gerontology by Unifesp and SBGG.
Address: 1061 Rui Barbosa st., Apy. 201. Centro. Zip code: 85851-170. Foz do Iguaçu-PR. Brazil
E-mail: cristinaribeiroft@gmail.com
-E-mail PhD Professor Mônica Sanches Yassuda: yassuda@usp.br
-E-mail PhD Professor Anita Liberalesso Neri: anitalbn@uol.com.br
RESUMO
Propósito de vida (PV) é definido como o senso de que a vida tem sentido e intencionalidade, o qual desempenha papel orientador em relação às metas de vida e à tomada de decisões relativas ao uso dos recursos pessoais. Objetivos: reconhecer e analisar dados sobre PV e condições identificadas com bom envelhecimento ou com adaptação positiva no envelhecimento. Método: foi realizada revisão integrativa de publicações de periódicos com avaliação por pares, indexadas nas bases de dados PubMed/Medline, Scopus, Web of Science, PsycInfo e Scielo/BVS/Lilacs, com os termos “purpose in life” and “aging” no título, sem restrição de datas. Resultados: foram selecionados 27 artigos, 22 dos quais conduzidos no âmbito de grandes estudos longitudinais prospectivos, 3 de corte transversal e 2 experimentais, que revelaram associações robustas entre alta pontuação em PV e redução do risco para mortalidade, Doença de Alzheimer, doenças coronarianas e cerebrovasculares, incapacidades e distúrbios de sono; alto PV e desfechos positivos em saúde, cognição, regulação emocional, bem-estar subjetivo e senso de ajustamento; PV como variável moderadora, ou recurso de resiliência, entre riscos e adversidades e boa adaptação. São dados úteis à investigação e à intervenção envolvendo idosos longevos e vulneráveis.
Palavras-chave: metas; satisfação pessoal; longevidade; envelhecimento
ABSTRACT
Purpose in life (PiL) is defined as the sense that life has meaning and intentionality, and plays a guiding role in life goals and decision making regarding the use of personal resources. Objectives: to recognize and analyze data on PiL and conditions associated with good aging or with positive adaptation in aging. Method: an integrative review of articles in peer-reviewed journals published in the PubMed/Medline, Scopus, Web of Science, PsycInfo and Scielo/BVS/Lilacs databases was performed using the terms "purpose in life" and "aging" in the title, without temporal restriction. Results: twenty-seven articles were selected, 22 of which were conducted under large prospective longitudinal studies, 3 were cross-sectional studies and 2 were experimental studies. The studies revealed robust associations between high PiL scores and lower risk of death, Alzheimer\'s disease, coronary and cerebrovascular diseases, disability, and sleep disorders; high PiL and positive outcomes in health, cognition, emotional regulation, subjective well-being, and sense of adjustment; PiL as a moderator or resilience resource between risks and adversities and good adaptation. These data are useful for research and intervention involving long-lived and vulnerable older persons.
Keywords: goals; personal satisfaction; longevity; aging
INTRODUCTION
In the 1980s and 1990s, the study of subjective well-being already attracted the interest of psychologists who were concerned with identifying variables associated with good quality of life in old age. The construct was described as composed of two sets of variables: global satisfaction with life and satisfaction related to positive and negative domains and affect. In 1989, psychologist Carol D. Ryff1 proposed that these aspects of subjective well-being were hedonic in nature and, although necessary, were insufficient to explain the adaptation of adults and older persons. Referring to Aristotelian philosophical tradition, the author argued that the eudaimonic aspect (eu = good + daimon = talent or potential) should also be considered. The pursuit of personal excellence would be relevant to the continuity of development and to the sense of adjustment of adults and older persons1-3.
Ryff operationalized psychological well-being (PWB) or eudaimonic well-being based on classical theoretical propositions on adult development (life structures/life goals4, individuation5, personal development/qualities of ego6, search for meaning7, maturity8, self-government9, self-fulfillment10, and executive processes of personality11, life-span theory12) and combined qualitative and quantitative strategies in a research program1-3; 13-15. This operationalization of PWB occurred through a scale based on six dimensions or factors: self-acceptance, personal growth, autonomy, environmental mastery, positive relationships with others, and purpose in life. In the original version1, the scale had 20 items in each domain, but subsequent versions produced instruments with 14 or fewer items, with good psychometric indicators of validity3; 13-15.
Theoretically, purpose in life (or simply purpose) is one of the two most important aspects to the definition of eudaimonic well-being (the other is personal growth). The construct was defined as a sense that life has meaning, direction and intentionality, and that this sense plays a guiding role in establishing life goals and decision making regarding the use of personal resources. Adults and older persons with a strong sense of purpose have life goals and a sense of direction, they feel that their past and current life is meaningful, and they have beliefs that give meaning to their lives, and have desires and goals that are worth living for1-3. The operationalization of the concept was made by one of the sub-scales of the measure of eudaimonic well-being. The Sub-Scale of Purpose had a first version with 14 items, and other versions with 3, 7 or 10 items3;15-16, but the most used is the one with 10 items16.
The term Purpose in Life (PiL) also makes reference to another tradition of research and practice, mainly the clinical one, which maintains the notions of directionality and intentionality but adds the concepts of existential meaning, belief in transcendence, and investment in the pursuit of ultimate end of existence. These components are seen as essential to the maintenance of psychological health, especially in life-threatening situations, loss of dignity, suffering and hopelessness, when they act as a resource of resilience, useful to promote adaptation in the face of adversity or of a significant risk7; 17-18.
Since the late 1980s, when the Ryff Scale1-3 was made public, research on purpose as an element of eudaimonic well-being grew more systematically and rapidly than research about the sense of meaning in philosophical-religious-psychological terms. Large ongoing longitudinal prospective cohort studies in the United States19-22 and another study in development in Australia23, started in the 2000s, have included among their objectives the investigation of associations between PiL and aspects of successful adaptation in adulthood and old age.
Research on conditions that help in the adaptation of older persons, as for example PiL, is of particular relevance in developing countries characterized by strong social inequality, inefficient health service networks and social security, and low levels of education and income of the population. Together, these factors determine the accumulation of disadvantages throughout life, which are potentiated by adversities and losses associated with aging. From the theoretical point of view, psychological variables, including PiL, can function as protective factors and as dispositional factors in relation to competence and self-fulfillment, self-care in health, cognition, activity and motivation for social participation. These possibilities are an incentive for conducting research and controlled interventions in samples of Brazilian elderly.
In the present work, PiL in old age it was chosen as object of study firstly because it has been suggested that this variable is likely associated with favorable outcomes in this phase of life. Subjective variables are recognized as important influences on physical and mental health, cognition and participation of the elderly, despite their disadvantages in terms of health or for socioeconomic reasons. Knowledge about the behavior of this variable among older persons can contribute to the planning of works oriented to the improvement of their psychological well-being. The other reason for this study is that it fills a gap in the production of knowledge about this phenomenon in old age in our country.
Literature reviews are a useful for gathering, relating, and synthesizing data from studies published on a given subject. They aid in the analysis of theoretical knowledge, help to make decisions about further research questions and about the methods to be adopted to answer them, as well as contribute to the planning of interventions relevant to the population. Integrative reviews are less restrictive than systematic reviews and meta-analyses that, based on theories, compare designs and numerical data24. They are well suited to start the empirical investigation of a given phenomenon, when the available theoretical propositions can serve as lenses to observe and intervene in reality.
We planned an integrative literature review with the goal to identify and analyze significant theoretical and empirical information about associations between PiL and conditions related to good aging or positive adaptation in aging.
METHOD
Integrative literature review is a method that synthesizes relevant information from available research on a given topic, and uses the product of such work to guide the practice and to plan further researches. It is organized in six phases: elaboration of the guiding question and the objective; systematic search through sampling or based on pre-established criteria according to the expected product; data collection; critical analysis; discussion of results; and presentation of the integrative review24.
Articles about purpose in life published in peer-reviewed journals were searched. The articles were indexed in the following databases: Medical Literature Analysis and Retrieval System Online (MEDLINE/PubMed); Virtual Library in Health, Latin American and Caribbean Health Sciences Literature, the Scientific Electronic Library Online (BVS/LILACS and SciELO); Scopus (Elsevier Science); Web of Science (Thompson Reuters) and the PsycInfo Data Base Record (American Psychological Association), with the descriptors "purpose in life" and "aging", without temporal restriction. The inclusion criteria were: research articles with sample of adults and older persons aged 50 and over, published in English, Portuguese or Spanish; articles that had the term "purpose in life" in the title and that adopted the definition of PiL in terms of directionality, intentionality and orientation by goals and objectives that give meaning to the choices and use of individual resources1-3. Articles whose definition integrated philosophical concepts of meaning, ultimate ends of existence, transcendence and religiosity (theoretical criteria) were excluded, as well as reviews and psychometric articles (methodological criteria). Articles that appeared in more than one database were considered only once. The searches were conducted in June 2016 and updated in May 2018.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
The initial search in the databases identified 387 titles that met the specification of the subject and the age of the participants (“Purpose in life” and “aging”). Of these, 87 were selected because they had the term "purpose in life" in the title and 30 were eliminated because they were repetitions. The resulting 57 articles had the abstract, introduction, instruments, and bibliography examined by two evaluators who worked first independently and then together to identify the underlying theoretical approach. A total of 32 articles were selected. They had a definition of PiL that involved the notions of directionality and goal-oriented behavior. Twenty-five articles were excluded because their definition of purpose included philosophical and religious elements. Further, two systematic reviews, one of meta-analysis and two of psychometric nature, were excluded. At the end, 27 articles were kept, read in full length, and used in the literature review. Figure 1 shows the flowchart that represents these actions and their results.
Figure 1
Twenty-two of the studies analyzed had been developed in the context of large longitudinal studies in the United States and one had been conducted in Australia: the Rush Memory and Aging Project (RMAP)19, the Health and Retirement Study (HRS)20, Midilife Development in the United States (MIDUS)21, the Veteran Affairs Normative Aging Study (NAS)22, and the Australian Longitudinal Study of Aging (ALSA)23. The inclusion of PiL among the measures of interest in these studies created excellent conditions of repetition, which have contributed to the increase of external validity of the results and of internal validity of PiL measures, and have contributed to theoretical advances.
The authorship of the articles is another indicative of integration between the studies. The authors can be grouped into four main blocks, led by researchers associated with longitudinal studies: in the RMAP, Boyle is the first author of four studies16,25-27 and coauthor in two28,29; in HRS, Kim is the first author of five publications29, 31-34; in the MIDUS, Hill and Turiano are responsible for four studies37, 39, 41, 42 and co-authors in two others42,52; and Burrow is the first author of two articles36,52. These and almost all other authors (n = 58 in the 27 articles) form groups with typical production. One article was published in 2009, two in 2010, one in 2011, one in 2012, three in 2013, four in 2014, six im 2015, four in 2016, two in 2017, and in from 2018.
The Rush Memory and Aging Project (RMAP)19 is a longitudinal, epidemiological, clinical-pathological, and cohort study that focuses on chronic conditions common in old age, with emphasis on cognitive and motor decline and risk of Alzheimer\'s disease. From September 1997 to November 2011, 1,556 adults and older persons recruited from retirement communities living in northeastern Illinois, United States, agreed to participate in the study. A total of 1,489 people completed the baseline measures (73.1% women). The mean age was 80.1 years and the mean schooling was 14.4 years. Among this sample, 1,409 did not present dementia in the clinical evaluation. The study focused on: (a) the relationship between risk factors and incident Alzheimer\'s disease (AD); (b) incident mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and decline in cognitive and motor functions; (c) the relationship between neurobiological indexes and AD, MCI and cognitive and motor functions; (d) the neurobiological trajectories linking risk factors and clinical phenotypes. Three follow-ups were performed involving postmortem neuropathological exams in the brains of the elderly who died.
The Health and Retirement Study (HRS)20 also involved a representative sample of men and women aged 50 and over. It began in 1991 as a longitudinal study of a cohort born between 1931 and 1941 in the pre-retirement period along with their spouses of any age. The sample was expanded in 1993 and 1998. Since then, every six years, a new cohort of people between the ages of 51 and 56 is included. Data are expected to be collected every four years. When a participant dies, an interview is conducted with family members to investigate the circumstances of the death and the socioeconomic conditions. Data on physical conditions, biomarkers, anthropometry, blood pressure, functional capacity, and psychosocial and socioeconomic variables are collected.
The Survey of Midlife Development in the United States (MIDUS)21 is a study of national scope. It involved 7,108 participants from 25 to 75 years of age at the baseline, which occurred in 1995 and 1996. The design of this study involves follow-up measures at intervals of 9 to 10 years. The aim is to investigate the role of behavioral, psychological and social factors in determining age-related variations in physical and mental health.
The Normative Aging Study (NAS)22 is a longitudinal interdisciplinary study established in 1961 by the Veterans Hospital Administration (Boston, USA). The cohort consisted of 2,280 men recruited from a universe of 6,000 examined at the beginning of the study, in 1961, when they were between 21 and 80 years of age. Most were white individuals, survivors of World War II and the Korean War. The only inclusion criterion was that they were healthy, i.e. free of any known chronic condition at the start of the study. Follow-ups were performed every 3 years until 1999. The focus of NAS is non-pathological aging, i.e., the relationships between normal aging and the natural history of chronic diseases. The parameters came from clinical medicine, biochemistry, the five senses, dental health, anthropometry and behavior.
The Australian Longitudinal Study of Aging (ALSA)23 included a sample of 2,087 men and women aged 65 years and older at baseline. The study sought to conduct a large prospective population survey whose data could be compared with those produced by studies in the United States. It began in 1992, with older persons randomly selected from Southern Australia\'s electoral records, stratified by sex, age (five cohorts), and location where they lived. Between 1992 and 2000, six waves of objective measures of successful old age indicators were conducted with respect to health, physical functionality, cognition, affectivity and personality.
The present review also included two studies of a prospective nature, with smaller, non-probabilistic samples, not integrated to the abovementioned longitudinal studies. One investigated the path of changes in PiL in middle-aged adults (n = 163) throughout five measurement waves of the Foley Longitudinal Study of Adulthood44. Another study was developed in a rural area of the province of Nara, Japan, with a total population of 6,900 people and looked for data on mortality in older persons who presented disability in IADL (instrumental activities of daily living) and BADL (basic activities of daily living). The participants were all 65 years old and older (n = 1,853) with valid answers in baseline measurements, performed by postal service45.
Box 1 presents information on the authors, objectives, countries, designs and participants of the 27 studies analyzed. In this table and in the following, the articles were first assembled according to methodological criteria (type of design) and, within each type, in chronological order.
Box 1
The measure of PiL used in 24 of the 27 studies was that constructed by Ryff and colleagues1-3;14;16 to function as one of the six sub-scales of their eudaimonic model of psychological well-being. The other three studies adopted measures compatible with the construct (asking if the elderly believed they had a motive and interests worth to live for)45, scale of achievement motives46, scale of involvement with life47, and scale of achievement of goals50. Two studies introduced as an additional measure the full scale of psychological well-being1-3; 33 and the sub-scale of positive relations with others1-3, 37-38. Box 2 summarizes the variables and measures used.
Box 2
A range of psychological variables was associated with measures of PiL, including positive affections and emotional self-regulation in laboratory situation35; evaluations of current, past and future satisfaction, which would protect the PiL of adults and older persons who have high and stable or increasing satisfaction and would negatively affect those with decreasing levels of satisfaction36; locus of control in health38; personality factors such as neuroticism (negative correlation with PiL), optimism and organization (positive correlations with PiL)16,42,43; depressive symptoms16,27,43 that correlate negatively with PiL; social participation, role expectations and achievement motive, whose correlations with PiL are positive37.
However, neither the advancement of age nor any other sociodemographic variables protected the participants of the study reported by Hill et al. (2018)41 from the deleterious effects of experiencing adversities before the age of 18, suggesting that there is a limit to the performance of subjective variables of a motivational nature, such as PiL. Purpose in life tends to decline with age42, reflecting the effect of losses associated with aging or social prejudice in relation to the provision of opportunities of achievement for older people. However, there appears to be a dispositional pattern of PiL which is stable and tends to persist for relatively long periods regardless of age44.
It was interesting to note that, with exception of age in some investigations, sociodemographic variables proved to be weak predictors of PiL. This data suggests that the phenomenon integrates a dimension that can be called structural or inherent to the human being, a notion that finds support in the theoretical foundations4-11 of the eudaimonic model1-3; 14-16 of psychological well-being in which PiL is inserted. The fact that older persons with a moderate level of dementia were able to respond to the sub-scale of purpose and were affected by the level of PiL strengthens this idea. Something that may have contributed to this result is the generally high educational level of participants in the American, Australian, and Japanese studies. In the study by Hill et al. (2016)39, income and net worth were significantly associated with PiL in the baseline measures and in subsequent follow-up measures, weakening the idea of PiL as a basic human need. These seemingly conflicting data need clarification. Data from Brazilian adults and elderly people with different levels of schooling and income and with different cognitive conditions from those observed in the countries of origin of these surveys may contribute to the debate.
The most impressive data observed in the set of studies analyzed concern the association between high level of PiL and better cognitive function, even despite the presence of neuropathological alterations typical of Alzheimer\'s disease, as well as slower evolution of AD and MCI incidents and slower rhythm of cognitive decline in the presence of high level of PiL27. These data suggest that PiL promotes gains in neural reserves that protect the brain from the deleterious effects of changes associated with AD. Most of the physiological systems of the body, including the brain, withstand a considerable level of damage before presenting clinical manifestations of deficit27.
Lower risk for AD in individuals with high PiL and statistically significant associations between high PiL scores and cognitive functions were also documented25,40,44. Adults and older persons with high PiL and good levels of global cognition and cognition in domains usually are more often involved in activities; they are competent in allocating time, energy and resources to reach their goals and usually invest more in their own health, which are conditions that benefit cognition. Further, there are relationships between PiL and reduced levels of inflammatory factors, such as IL-6 receptor, and between PiL and greater resistance to stress and better immune function, which are influenced by the indicated factors53. Individuals with good biomarkers, low allostatic load, and more cognitive resources invest more personal resources in goals that benefit the acquisition of new repertoires and subjective well-being38,40.
The second set of data that draws attention in the literature sample analyzed concerns the capacity of PiL to predict mortality. The studies of Boyle et al. (2009)16, Hill and Turiano (2014)37, Windsor et al. (2015)43 and Tomioka et al. (2016)45 assumed survival and mortality as dependent variables. In the first study, high PiL was associated with a higher probability of survival; the second indicated a higher risk of death for male, older and less educated elderly people and those who scored low on positive relationships with others and PiL; the third showed an increase in age as a risk factor for mortality and an increase in the PiL score as a predictor of survival; the last, associated low PiL scores with mortality and with decline in IADL and BADL. The biological bases of the association between PiL and mortality are unknown, but it is believed that high levels of PiL contribute to the functioning of multiple biological systems, protecting them from the deleterious effects of diseases and other adverse health conditions, using the same reasoning presented with regard to the incidence and progression of AD and MCI.
Studies of the potential of PiL to predict stroke and myocardial infarction, which are themselves predictors of physical and cognitive impairment, is one of the preferred goals of clinical practice and research, hence the importance of data reported by Yu et al. (2015)28 and by Kim et al. (2013a and 2013b)30-31. According to the authors, high PiL scores are associated with lower risk for stroke and for myocardial infarction. There are two possible explanations for the associations between PiL and stroke. First of all, PiL can help by promoting healthy lifestyles. Second, PiL may be directly implicated in neuroendocrine functions, for example levels of salivary cortisol, epinephrine and norepinephrine, which are related to subjective well-being. Another possible mechanism includes the action of inflammatory markers, such as C-reactive protein, IL-6, and tumor necrosis factor alpha. Third, PiL relates to several other psychological constructs, such as sense of coherence, sense of control, resilience, and optimism. It is a component of psychological well-being and a potentially modifiable factor promoting healthy aging28,31.
The simple absence of negative psychological status and traits such as anxiety, hostility, depression and negative affections is insufficient to explain or prevent the occurrence of myocardial infarctions. Protein mechanisms may travel along biological (e.g. promoting better immune system function and better regulation of HDL levels), behavioral (e.g., optimism, positive emotions, self-efficacy, and problem-focused coping) and social (e.g., positive social relationships and participation in community affairs) trajectories, all of which are associated with high levels of PiL30.
High levels of PiL were associated with a lower risk of disability in BADL and mobility, regardless of sociodemographic variables, fragility, cardiovascular risk, vascular diseases, depression, neuroticism, and social networks26. In the study by Kim et al. (2017)34, each additional point in PiL was associated with a 13% decrease in the risk of developing low grip strength and with 14% decrease in the risk of presenting slow gait. There is insufficient data linking PiL and physical functions. Relationships are believed to be based in part on indirect effects (e.g., through health behaviors), and in part on direct effects (e.g., through change in biological functions). Thus, people with high levels of PiL are more proactive in health care, have better impulse control, and are more likely to engage in healthy activities. They have a better level of regulation of the physiological systems associated with physical functions (e.g., fewer inflammatory markers). These conditions mediate the relationship between PiL and functional capacity34.
Windsor et al. (2015)43 and Tomioka et al. (2016)45 confirmed these associations with similar data. Zilioli et al. (2015)38 observed better conditions to cope with stressors during a 10-year follow-up of elderly with high level of PiL. These advantages probably reflect and explain the occurrence of positive health assessments in the presence of high PiL levels42-43.
High PiL in elderly patients in outpatient care predicts lower hospitalization rates than intermediate levels of PiL29. Further, higher PiL is associated with a greater probability of engaging in measures of self-care in health32. Unlike the positive effects related to higher scores on PiL, lower scores predict sleep disorders33 and other negative outcomes29.
Box 3
The cross-sectional studies involved convenience samples and focused on theoretical questions46,47 and validity of physical activity measures in relation to PiL48. One of the three experimental studies analyzed involved a sample of MIDUS35 and was guided by the theory of social-emotional selectivity. According to the theory, aging implies an adaptive decline in intensity, variability and reaction time related to emotional expression, as well as a decline in the ability to discriminate signs of negative emotions in photographs and speech. The decline would be protective by allowing the elderly to save physiological resources to invest in selected domains, such as cognition. At the same time, elderly people retain the ability to perceive facial expressions of fear, even in photographs of crowds, possibly because of their adaptive value for the species49. The intervention was important because it documented the possibility of intervening in PiL or in conditions associated with it. The second was guided by the notion of goal-oriented behavior as a fundamental strategy for the management of external and internal stressors50. Such a notion is inherent in the concept of PiL, but the novelty of the study was to test the possibility of communication with older persons with dementia regarding PiL and to effectively know if PiL affects their behaviors. Both endeavors were successful, providing important data to interventions with adults and older persons with dementia51. The third experimental study was different from the others due to the sensitive theme, attitudes regarding ethnic diversity in relation to other affective variables, including PiL, and cognitive variables52. The study was a relevant attempt to test the possibility of intervening in these conditions, in favor of society (see Box 4).
Box 4
FINAL CONSIDERATIONS
We performed an integrative review of literature on PiL that aimed to carry out the analysis of a sample of articles selected by theoretical and methodological criteria. In the decision to select articles that include the term in the title, we intended to focus on studies considered significant in the contexts in which they were produced and published. The vast majority of articles analyzed worked with secondary data or with sub-samples from large studies conducted in the United States, within the tradition of longitudinal studies. All are pioneers in the relationship between PiL and numerous biological, psychological and social variables indicative of good adaptation in old age.
The aging of the population and the increasing longevity in Brazil associated with normative and non-normative changes that occur in old age call attention to conditions of risk to adaptation, which generate the need to study variables that favor the well-being of the elderly and successful aging. Among them, the most important are those that can be modified, as for example psychological or eudaimonic well-being and one of its components, the purpose of life. For these reasons, the ultimate objective assumed by this work is to offer researchers and health professionals an analysis on relevant aspects of this construct, which can assist them in decision making during interventions.
In short, the data analyzed indicate that the tendency to extract meaning from life experiences and to have a sense of intentionality and direction can guide the behavior, contributing to successful aging. Adults and older persons with high levels of purpose in life, whose work is oriented by goals in life, face aging better, invest more in self-care and in developing their own skills, engage more socially, and tend to participate in meaningful activities. People with a higher level of purpose have a more positive view of aging and of their lives.
People with higher levels of PiL tend to be more resilient and their active pursuit of goals is likely to increase their goal-oriented strength and the efficiency of their biological systems. PiL is associated with numerous known psychological states and traits that can provide good health, such as optimism, self-efficacy, and absence of depression. It is associated with behaviors compatible with health preservation such as healthy eating, and regular physical activity, and avoidance of risks such as heavy alcohol consumption or smoking.
PiL acts as a significant protection factor in relation to mortality; cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases; cognitive changes compatible with AD and MCI; functional limitation and immobility; hospitalizations; and other negative health outcomes. It is relatively little affected by sociodemographic variables. It is stable throughout the ages, in adult life and in old age. Although it is a promoter of successful development and aging, the scope of its action to modify and prevent risks associated with aging is limited by the process of decline in more advanced old age.
Purpose can be improved by engaging in meaningful activities such as leisure, educational, philanthropic, social, cultural or family activities. Even small behavioral changes can contribute to a greater sense of purpose, usefulness and relevance. Interventions focused on the improvement of PiL in the elderly can contribute to strengthen neurological and cognitive reserves, the sense of control, competence and goal-oriented behavior. They may be of great value in attenuating, delaying, and slowing down the rate of cognitive decline and the development of morbidity and disability in old age.
Increased longevity among Brazilian elderly imposes challenges to society and aging individuals. The possibility of loss of physical and cognitive capacities and of experiencing individual and family adversities increases in advanced old ages. Gerontological research should produce knowledge about variables that can contribute to healthy longevity. PiL stands out as a significant marker of physical and mental health, with potential to guide innovative gerontological interventions.
REFERENCES
1. Ryff CD. Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol.1989;57(6):1069–81.
2. Ryff CD. Psychological well-being in adult life. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 1995;4(4):99–104.
3. Ryff CD, Keyes CL. The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J Pers Soc Psychol.1995;69(4):719-27.
4. Bühler C. The curve of life as studied in biographies. J Appl Psychol. 1935;19(4):405-9.
5. Jung CG. Modern man in search of a soul. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World; 1933.
6. Erikson EH. Identity and the life cycle: Selected papers. Psychological Issues. 1959;1:1-171.
7. Frankl VE. Man\'s search for meaning: An introduction to logotherapy. Boston, MA: Beacon Press; 1959.
8. Allport GW. Pattern and growth in personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston; 1961.
9. Rogers CR. On becoming a person. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin; 1961.
10. Maslow AH. Toward a psychology of being. 2 ed. New York: Van Nostrand; 1968.
11. Neugarten BL. Personality change in late life: A developmental perspective. In: Eisodorfer C, Lawton MP, editors. The psychology of adult development and aging. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.;1973. p. 311-35.
12. Baltes PB, Baltes MM. Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In: Baltes MM, Baltes PB, editors. Successful Aging: Perspectives from the Behavioral Sciences. European Network on Longitudinal Studies on Individual Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press;1990. p. 1-34.
13. Samman E. Psychological and subjective well-being: A proposal for internationally comparable indicators. Oxford Development Studies. 2007;35(4):459-86.
14. Abbott RA, Ploubidis GB, Huppert FA, Kuh D, Wadsworth ME, Croudace TJ. Psychometric evaluation and predictive validity of Ryff\'s psychological well-being items in a UK birth cohort sample of women. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2006;4(1):76.
15. Ryff CD, Singer BH. Best news yet on the six-factor model of well-being. Social Science Research.2006;35(4):1103-19.
16. Boyle PA, Barnes LL, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Purpose in life is associated with mortality among community-dwelling older persons. Psychosomatic medicine.2009;71(5):574-9.
17. Koenig HG, Berk LS, Daher NS, Pearce MJ, Bellinger DL, Robins CJ, et al. Religious involvement is associated with greater purpose, optimism, generosity and gratitude in persons with major depression and chronic medical illness. J Psychosom Res.2014;77(2):135-43.
18. Wong PTP. Spirituality, meaning, and successful aging. The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological research and clinical applications. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers;1998. p. 359-94.
19. Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, Boyle PA,Wilson, RS. Overview and findings from the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Curr Alzheimer Res.2012; 9(6): 646–663.
20. Juster, FT, Suzman R. An overview of the Health and Retirement Study. J Hum Res.30;Special Issue on the Health and Retirement Study: Data quality and early results.1995; p. S7-S56.
21. Brim OG, Ryff CD, Kessler RC (2004). The MIDUS National Survey: An Overview. In OG Brim, CD Ryff, R C Kessler (Eds.), The John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur foundation series on mental health and development. Studies on successful midlife development. How healthy are we?: A national study of well-being at midlife. Chicago, IL, US: University of Chicago Press;2004. p.1-34.
22. Bell B, Rose CL, Damon A. The Normative Aging Study. An Interdisciplinary and Longitudinal Study of Health and Aging. Aging Hum Devel.1972;3(1): 5-17
23. Andrews G, Cheok F, Carr S. The Australian Longitudinal Study of Aging. Austr J Age.1989;8(2):31-35.
24. Souza MTS, Silva MD, Carvalho R. Revisão integrativa: o que é e como fazer. Einstein.2017;8(1 Pt 1):102-6.
25. Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA. Effect of a purpose in life on risk of incident Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older persons. Arch Gen Psychiatry.2010;67(3):304-10.
26. Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Purpose in life is associated with a reduced risk of incident disability among community-dwelling older persons. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry.2010;18(12):1093-102.
27. Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Wilson RS, Yu L, Schneider JA, Bennett DA. Effect of purpose in life on the relation between Alzheimer disease pathologic changes on cognitive function in advanced age. Arch Gen Psychiatry.2012;69(5):499-505.
28. Yu L, Boyle PA, Wilson RS, Levine SR, Schneider JA, Bennett DA. Purpose in life and cerebral infarcts in community-dwelling older people. Stroke. 2015;46(4):1071-6.
29. Wilson RS, Capuano AW, James BD, Amofa P, Arvanitakis Z, Shah R, et al. Purpose in life and hospitalization for ambulatory care-sensitive conditions in old age. Amer J Ger Psych.2018;26(3):364-74.
30. Kim ES, Sun JK, Park N, Kubzansky LD, Peterson C. Purpose in life and reduced risk of myocardial infarction among older U.S. adults with coronary heart disease: a two-year follow-up. J Behav Med.2013;36(2):124-33.
31. Kim ES, Sun JK, Park N, Peterson C. Purpose in life and reduced incidence of stroke in older adults: \'The Health and Retirement Study\'.J Psychosom Res. 2013;74(5):427-32.
32. Kim ES, Strecher VJ, Ryff CD. Purpose in life and use of preventive health care services. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A.2014;111(46):16331-6.
33. Kim ES, Hershner SD, Strecher VJ. Purpose in life and incidence of sleep disturbances. J Behav Med. 2015;38(3):590-7.
34. Kim ES, Kawachi I, Chen Y, Kubzansky LD. Association between purpose in life and objective measures of physical function in older adults. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(10):1039-45.
35. Schaefer SM, Morozink Boylan J, van Reekum CM, Lapate RC, Norris CJ, Ryff CD, et al. Purpose in life predicts better emotional recovery from negative stimuli. PloS one.2013;8(11):e80329.
36. Burrow AL, Sumner R, Ong AD. Perceived change in life satisfaction and daily negative affect: The moderating role of purpose in life. J Happiness Stud. 2014;15(3):579-92.
37. Hill PL, Turiano NA. Purpose in life as a predictor of mortality across adulthood. Psychol Sci.2014;25(7):1482-6.
38. Zilioli S, Slatcher RB, Ong AD, Gruenewald TL. Purpose in life predicts allostatic load ten years later. J Psychosom Res.2015;79(5):451-7.
39. Hill PL, Turiano NA, Mroczek DK, Burrow AL. The value of a purposeful life: Sense of purpose predicts greater income and net worth. J Res Pers.2016;65:38-42.
40. Lewis NA, Turiano NA, Payne BR, Hill PL. Purpose in life and cognitive functioning in adulthood. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2017;24(6):662-71.
41. Hill PL, Turiano NA, Burrow L. Early life adversity as a predictor of sense of purpose during adulthood. Int J Behav Dev.2018;42(1):143–7.
42. Hill PL, Turiano NA, Spiro A, Mroczek DK. Understanding inter-individual variability in purpose in life: Longitudinal findings from the VA Normative Aging Study. Psychol Aging.2015;30(3):529-33.
43. Windsor TD, Curtis RG, Luszcz MA. Sense of purpose as a psychological resource for aging well. Devel Psych.2015;51(7):975-86.
44. Ko HJ, Hooker K, Geldhof GJ, McAdams DP. Longitudinal purpose in life trajectories: Examining predictors in late midlife. Psychol Aging. 2016;31(7):693-8.
45. Tomioka K, Kurumatani N, Hosoi H. Relationship of having hobbies and a purpose in life with mortality, activities of daily living, and instrumental activities of daily living among community-dwelling elderly adults. J Epidemiol. 2016;26(7):361-70.
46. Sano N, Kyougoku M. An analysis of structural relationship among achievement motive on social participation, purpose in life, and role expectations among community dwelling elderly attending day services. PeerJ.2016;4:e1655.
47. Polenick CA, Kales HC, Birditt KS. Perceptions of purpose in life within spousal care dyads: Associations with emotional and physical caregiving difficulties. Ann Behav Med.2018;52(1):77-87.
48. Hooker SA, Masters KS. Purpose in life is associated with physical activity measured by accelerometer. J Health Psychol.2016;21(6):962-71.
49. Charles ST. Emotional experience and regulation in later life. In KW Schaie, SL Willis (Eds). Handbook of the Psychology of Aging 7th edition. London: Elsevier;2011.p.295-310.
50. Brandtstädter J, Renner G. Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment: Explication and age-related analysis of assimilative and accommodative strategies of coping. Psychol Aging;1990;5:58–67.
51. Mak W. Self-reported goal pursuit and purpose in life among people with dementia. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci.2011;66(2):177-84.
52. Burrow AL, Stanley M, Sumner R, Hill PL. Purpose in life as a resource for increasing comfort with ethnic diversity. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2014;40(11):1507-16.
53. Friedman EM, Hayney M, Love GD, Singer BH, Ryff CD. Plasma interleukin-6 and soluble IL-6 receptors are associated with psychological well-being in aging women. Healt Psych.2007;26(3): 305–313.